Indiana University

Skip to:

  1. Search
  2. Breadcrumb Navigation
  3. Content
  4. Browse by Topic
  5. Services & Resources
  6. Additional Resources
  7. Multimedia News

Media Contacts

James Devitt
New York University
james.devitt@nyu.edu
212-998-6808

Tracy James
Indiana University
traljame@indiana.edu
812-855-0084

Last modified: Wednesday, October 14, 2009

Momentum influences baby name choices, cognitive scientists find

FOR IMMEDIATE RELEASE
Oct. 14, 2009

NEW YORK -- Like momentum traders in the stock market, parents today appear to favor names that recently have risen in popularity relative to names that are on the decline, say cognitive science researchers from Indiana University and New York University.

baby

Photo by Chris Meyer

Names rising in popularity catch parents' attention

Researchers have long noted that the overall popularity of a baby name exerts a strong influence on parents' preferences -- more popular names, such as Robert or Susan, are more frequent, and by their sheer ubiquity, drive more parents to adopt a similar choice. However, a new study by Todd Gureckis, assistant professor of psychology at New York University, and Robert Goldstone, director of the Cognitive Science Program at IU, suggests that the change in popularity of a name over time increasingly influences naming decisions in the United States.

"Parents in the United States are increasingly sensitive to the change in frequency of a name in recent time, such that names that are gaining in popularity are seen as more desirable than those that have fallen in popularity in the recent past," the authors noted. "This bias then becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy: names that are falling continue to fall while names on the rise reach new heights of popularity, in turn influencing a new generation of parents."

Robert Goldstone

Robert Goldstone

Print-Quality Photo

The research, which was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health and the National Science Foundation, is relevant to understanding how people's everyday decisions are influenced by aggregate cultural processes.

"Our results give support to the idea that individual naming choices are in a large part determined by the social environment that expecting parents experience," the authors wrote in Topics of Cognitive Science. "Like the stock market, cycles of boom and bust appear to arise out of the interactions of a large set of agents who are continually influencing one another."

The researchers note this pattern is a relatively new phenomenon. In the late 19th and early 20th centuries, the popularity of a name from one year to the next was correlated with a decrease in future popularity. The changing pattern, the authors suggest, arises from biases in how people estimate the overall desirability of cultural tokens like names. That is, tokens that are recently outpacing their long-term popularity are seen as better choices than those that appear to be falling out of favor.

The findings were based on a historical record of the frequency that particular names were given to babies during the last 127 years in the United States provided by the U.S. Social Security Administration.

Existing accounts of cultural evolution suggest that it is primarily the frequency of the token (i.e., name) in a parent's social environment that should drive aggregate patterns of name choice. However, by sorting through names and watching the way they rise and fall in popularity over time, the authors noted that many names appear to take surprisingly smooth trajectories through time such that increasing popularity one year is often associated with increasing popularity the next. Moreover, this trend has become more pronounced over the years.

In order to better quantify this effect, the authors analyzed the probability that a name goes up or down from one year to the next, given that it went up or down in the time period before. They found that around the turn of the last century (1880 to 1905) names tended to fluctuate in overall frequency from one year to the next. A name that increased its relative frequency one year was more likely to decrease rather than increase in frequency the following year. Similarly, decreases in frequency were more likely to be followed by increases than further decreases.

More recently (1981 to 2006), names moved in consistent ways such that a change in popularity in one year was predictive of the same direction of change the following year. Thus, names appear to carry with them a "momentum" that tends to push changes in popularity in the same direction year after year.

In the paper, the authors develop and test a number of formal models of cultural evolution in order to quantify the sources of bias that influence people's naming decisions. In particular, the authors incorporated well-known aspects of cognitive processing, including the way that novelty and familiarity bias our preferences. The authors found that a model that assumes that names which are outpacing their long-term popularity are preferentially selected better explains the distribution of names over time than do models which leave out this assumption.

The researchers argue that baby names provide a unique opportunity for studying the intersection of individual and group decision making for the following reasons:

  • It's an important decision upon which parents devote significant time and energy;
  • there are extensive historical records, making possible the detailed measurement of these choices and the social context in which those decisions were made;
  • certain names (e.g., "Joshua", a popular name in 2007) do not appear to carry more intrinsic economic value than other names (e.g., "Damarion," an uncommon boys name the same year); and
  • baby names are not subject to the forces of marketing or advertising -- factors that may complicate the analysis of other type of culturally-relevant decisions such as fashion or music preferences..